Brigham Young University
BYU ScholarsArchive
**7$0$0 ,#(00$/1 1(-,0

Social Media Use and Its Impact on Relationships
and Emotions
Spencer Palmer Christensen
Brigham Young University
-**-41'(0 ,# ##(1(-, *4-/)0 1 '8.00"'-* /0 /"'(3$!52$#2$1#
/1-%1'$ -++2,(" 1(-,-++-,0
7(07$0(0(0!/-2&'11-5-2%-/%/$$ ,#-.$, ""$00!5"'-* /0/"'(3$1' 0!$$, ""$.1$#%-/(,"*20(-,(,**7$0$0 ,#(00$/1 1(-,0!5 ,
21'-/(6$# #+(,(01/ 1-/-%"'-* /0/"'(3$-/+-/$(,%-/+ 1(-,.*$ 0$"-,1 "1 0"'-* /0 /"'(3$!52$#2$**$, + 1 ,&$*-!52$#2
"'-* /0/"'(3$(1 1(-,
'/(01$,0$,.$,"$/ *+$/-"( *$#( 0$ ,#10+. "1-,$* 1(-,0'(.0 ,#+-1(-,0 All eses and Dissertations

'8.00"'-* /0 /"'(3$!52$#2$1#
Social Media Use and Its Impact on Relationships and Emotions
Spencer Palmer Christensen
A thesis submitted to the faculty of
Brigham Young University
in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of
Master of Arts
Kristoffer D. Boyle, Chair
Scott H. Church
Robert I. Wakefield
School of Communications
Brigham Young University
Copyright © 2018 Spencer Palmer Christensen
All Rights Reserved
ABSTRACT
Social Media Use and Its Impact on Relationships and Emotions
Spencer Palmer Christensen
School of Communications, BYU
Master of Arts
A large majority of the people throughout the world own a smartphone and access social media
on a daily basis. Because of this digital attachment, the author sought to understand to what
extent this use has impacted the users’ emotional well-being and offline interpersonal
relationships. A sample size of 627 participants completed a mixed-methods survey consisting of
Likert scale and short answer questions regarding social media use, emotional well-being and
interpersonal relationships. Results revealed that the more time an individual spent on social
media the more likely they were to experience a negative impact on their overall emotional well-
being and decreased quality in their relationships. Emotional well-being also mediated the
relationship between time spent using social media and the quality of that user’s relationships,
meaning that the more time a person spent on social media the more likely their emotional well-
being declined which then negatively impacted their relationships. The top three responses for
negative effects of social media use on emotions were frustration, depression, and social
comparison. The top three responses for negative effects of social media use on interpersonal
relationships were distraction, irritation, and decreased quality time with their significant other in
offline settings. An analysis of these, and other, results, along with relative implications, are
discussed.
Keywords: social media, emotional well-being, interpersonal relationships, uses and
gratifications
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
I want to personally thank everyone who has contributed to helping me complete my
thesis, without whom I would not have met the requirements for graduation. My wife, Kaily, has
been my champion and supporter through all of graduate school and I thank her for her love and
companionship. My daughter, Evelyn, served as my motivation to push through the hard times so
that I could support her. To my committee: Kris Boyle, Scott Church, and Robert Wakefield, I
thank you for your advice, insights, and counsel that has shaped my thesis into what it has
become; I could not have made it this far without your help. Lastly, to Chris Wilson, Kevin John,
and Jared Hansen, I thank you for your help in organizing and analyzing my quantitative results
as my knowledge of SPSS was very limited.
iv
TABLE OF CONTENTS
TITLE PAGE .............................................................................................................................. i
ABSTRACT .............................................................................................................................. ii
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ......................................................................................................... iii
TABLE OF CONTENTS .......................................................................................................... iv
Introduction ................................................................................................................................1
Literature Review ........................................................................................................................3
Uses and Gratifications Theory ...............................................................................................3
Benefits of Social Media .........................................................................................................4
Interpersonal Relationships in a Digital Age ............................................................................8
FOMO and Anxiety ............................................................................................................... 10
Depression and Loneliness .................................................................................................... 13
Research Questions/Hypotheses ............................................................................................ 15
Method ..................................................................................................................................... 16
Results ...................................................................................................................................... 23
Figure 1 ................................................................................................................................. 26
Figure 2 ................................................................................................................................. 27
Discussion ................................................................................................................................. 32
Conclusion ................................................................................................................................ 45
References ................................................................................................................................ 47
v
Appendix ASocial Media Survey .......................................................................................... 54
Appendix BTables ................................................................................................................. 58
Table 1 .................................................................................................................................. 58
Table 2 .................................................................................................................................. 58
Table 3 .................................................................................................................................. 58
Table 4 .................................................................................................................................. 60
Table 5 .................................................................................................................................. 60
Table 6 .................................................................................................................................. 60
Table 7 .................................................................................................................................. 61
Table 8 .................................................................................................................................. 62
Table 9 .................................................................................................................................. 62
Table 10 ................................................................................................................................ 63
Table 11 ................................................................................................................................ 63
Table 12 ................................................................................................................................ 64
Table 13 ................................................................................................................................ 65
Table 14 ................................................................................................................................ 66
Table 15 ................................................................................................................................ 67
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 1
Introduction
Social media use is a ubiquitous phenomenon (Elhai, Levine, Dvorak, & Hall, 2016;
Pittman, & Reich, 2016; Quinn, 2016). Research shows that 90% of adults own a smartphone
(Pew Research Center, 2014). Additional research indicates that 72% of Americans and an
average of 43% of the world own a smartphone (Elhai et al., 2016) while more than 71% of
American adolescents, ages 13-17, regularly use Facebook (Beyens, Frison, & Eggermont,
2016). Facebook is the most popular social networking site in the world containing 1.5 billion
active users with at least 900 million of these logging into the site daily (Ryan, Chester, Reece, &
Xenos, 2014). Pittman and Reich (2016) synthesized these findings to indicate that 91% of
smartphone owners used social networking sites on their phone at least once every day.
Due to the prevalence of social media in our lives, the people of the world are more
interconnected than at any other time in history. Because of this, there could be a perception that
people are happier because they are connected with more people. In fact, Nezlek, Richardson,
Green, and Schatten-Jones (2002) found that participants who were more socially active [offline]
reported greater life satisfaction and higher psychological well-being. However, social
interaction in the virtual world tells a different story, especially when those online connections
impact our offline interpersonal relationships.
Throughout the past decade, social media use has grown exponentially and has changed
the way we communicate with each other. Facebook is the most used online media platform in
the world (Beyens, Frison, & Eggermont, 2016; Steers, 2016) and has a high potential for
impacting the emotions and relationships of adolescents who use it (Kross et al., 2013). The
primary purpose of this paper is to determine if a relationship exists between excessive social
media use and the overall emotional well-being of that individual as well as the quality of the
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 2
individual’s interpersonal relationships. The secondary purpose is to determine if the relationship
between time spent on social media and the quality of the interpersonal relationships is mediated
by the emotional well-being of the user such as fear of missing out (FOMO), anxiety, depression,
and loneliness as seen through the lens of uses and gratifications theory.
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 3
Literature Review
Uses and Gratifications Theory
The most common theory used to understand why people engage with social media is
uses and gratifications theory (U&G). This theory was first proposed by Elihu Katz and his
partners Jay Blumler and Michael Gurevitch in 1973 and was used to study the motives people
have for engaging with the media that they do in order to gratify their needs (Katz, Blumler, &
Gurevitch, 1973). U&G is a psychological communication perspective and theorizes that
individuals are actively engaged in seeking out media that they believe will satisfy certain needs
(Katz et al., 1973; Rubin, 2009). U&G posits that media consumers make their own choices on
which media and what type of media they consume in order to receive maximum gratification for
their needs (Alajmi et al., 2016). To summarize, U&G focuses on consumersmotives for using
specific types of media and the satisfaction they receive from their use.
People make their own decisions on which media to engage with in order to satisfy their
needs, however those needs are not always obtained. Often times, the gratifications sought are
not the same as the gratifications obtained and although strongly correlated, continued use of a
medium over time implies that the gratifications obtained strongly reinforce continued use of that
same medium in order to continue seeking the gratifications originally sought after (Levy &
Windahl, 1984; Palmgreen, Wenner, & Rayburn, 1980).
Blumler and Katz (1974) synthesized U&G by explaining that is was focused on social
and psychological needs that create certain expectations of mass media which lead to particular
patterns of media exposure and result in need gratification as well as other consequences,
although these other consequences are perhaps unintended. Blumler et al. (1974) further
explained that there were five main components to U&G:
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 4
1. The audience is believed to be active
2. The linking of gratification and media choice lies with the consumer
3. The media compete with other sources of gratification
4. The goals of mass media are derived from the content created by the consumers
5. Value judgments of mass media should be suspended while consumer orientations are
explored
While uses and gratifications theory was once used to explore the gratifications gained
from TV and radio use, it has since been adapted for the study of social media and its various
elements such as gratifications from Facebook use (Park, Kee, & Valenzuela, 2009; Quan-Haase
& Young, 2010), privacy regulations online (Quinn, 2016), Chinese social media apps (Gan,
2018), social capital (Petersen & Johnston, 2015), and motivations for social media use (Cheung,
Chiu, & Lee, 2011), among others which all contribute to the credibility of using this theory for
the purposes of the present study. Further evidence supporting the use of this theory to study
social media is that the five main components of U&G proposed by Katz et al. (1974) can be
applied to social media use. U&G is widely considered a pro-social theory that highlights the
benefits for using various types of media and some of those benefits are worth taking the time to
examine.
Benefits of Social Media
With a large portion of the world accessing social media on a daily basis, there is ever-
increasing evidence that social media offer a varied experience for each user and that some of
those experiences produce positive results. These benefits offer possible explanations as to why
social media usage is continuing to grow throughout the world. One of the most common reasons
that people use social media is to stay connected with their friends and family members
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 5
(Pempek, Yermolayeva, & Calvert, 2009; Subrahmanyam, Reich, Waechter, & Espinoza, 2008;
Wang, Tchernev, & Solloway, 2012). Social media offer an easy way of keeping in touch and
maintaining relationships with people who are often beyond the close proximity of frequent
communication. Subrahmanyam et al. (2008) added to this by suggesting that many social media
users use it to both connect and reconnect with others indicating that there was overlap between
participants' online and offline networks. However, the overlap was imperfect; the pattern
suggested that many online users engaged in different online contexts to manage various parts of
their offline connections.
Online profiles often reflect some version of the offline lives they represent. In these
online profiles, social media users express certain elements of themselves that they want others
to see. In other words, the user manipulates the preferences of their profile to build an online
identity (Pempek et al., 2009). In addition to helping establish an online identity, social media
use also offers gratification in certain emotional, cognitive, social, and habitual areas of the
users’ lives (Wang et al., 2012). Generally however, only some of these areas are fully gratified
leading to an accumulation of ungratified needs which drives subsequent social media use and
contributes to the user becoming addicted or, at the very least, using social media excessively
unless those needs are satisfied in offline situations.
Desired gratifications on social media often drive the behaviors that lead to those
gratifications. Hayes, Carr, and Wohn (2016) explored the meaning that “liking” a post on
Facebook (or a “favorite” on Twitter, etc…) had for both the original poster and the one who
“liked” the post. The results of the study indicated that people devalued Facebook “likes owing
to the fact that they were more reactionary than conscious. Favorites on Twitter did not matter
because it was more about the content than the social capital. Liking on Instagram was more
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 6
selective. Receiving upvotes on Reddit contributed to the social currency of the post making it
more trustworthy and accepted by other redditors.
Additionally, findings from the study by Hayes et al. (2016) revealed four main
motivations for sending a paralinguistic digital affordance (PDAaka “liking,” or “favoriting” a
post) and three main gratifications for receiving a PDA. The motivations for sending a PDA
included: literal interpretationthe PDA was an evaluation of the content; acknowledgement of
viewingthe PDA served as an acknowledgement to the poster that they had seen the post;
social supportthe PDA served as a way of saying that you supported the person in their
endeavors; and lastly utilitarian purposesthe PDA served as a personal score card to make
themselves feel better about sending out so many PDA’s to so many people. On the flip side,
those who originally sent the post received three main gratifications from PDA’s: emotional
gratificationparticipants reported feeling happy when they received a PDA; status
gratificationthe more PDA’s their post received the higher their social status; social
gratificationPDA’s served to create or enhance interpersonal relationships (Hayes et al., 2016).
The results from Hayes et al. (2016) explained that there were various gratifications
people received from using social media. However, additional research will help to further
illuminate this phenomenon. Oeldorf-Hirsch and Sundar (2016) explored motivations for why
people share photos online. The participants were asked questions regarding why they share
photos online and the results revealed four categories of gratifications: seeking and
showcasingthe need to keep up with the world and keep tabs on others; technological
affordancesthe features of the platform make it easier to share; social connection
maintaining close relationships and creating new relationships; and reaching outwanting to
reach a wide audience and receive feedback on their photos. These findings indicate that photo
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 7
sharing is driven by social needs and that the platform offered special affordances that enabled
the behavior (Oeldorf-Hirsch & Sundar, 2016).
Interactions on social media have frequently been referred to as bridging and bonding
social capital (Ellison, Steinfield, & Lampe, 2007; Putnam, 2000). Bourdieu and Wacquant
(1992) define social capital as “the sum of the resources, actual or virtual, that accrue to an
individual or a group by virtue of possessing a durable network of more or less institutionalized
relationships of mutual acquaintance and recognition” (p. 14). As it relates to social media, social
capital is the relationships established online that enrich virtual interactions. Bridging and
bonding are often placed as opposites to each other, but this would be an incorrect assumption of
these concepts. Rather, they are relatable dimensions along which different forms of social
capital can be compared. Bridging social capital is composed of several elements including:
connecting with people who think differently from ‘me;’ ties are generally weaker and more
fragile, but they allow for more open doors that bonding does not allow; more likely to foster
social inclusion; good for linking external assets and information diffusion; good for getting
ahead; and can generate a broader range of identities. Examples include: loose connections,
lesser-known classmates at school, LinkedIn connections, and your brother’s boss, among others.
Bonding social capital is comparable, but with key differences: connecting with like-minded
individuals; ties are stronger and are usually kept within a smaller circle of connections; fosters
social exclusion due to strong in-group loyalty; good for getting by; and can be referred to as an
echo chamber of individuals who think alike without opposing ideas. Examples include: families,
closed group forums, and fraternities (Putnam, 2000).
When a user engages with others on social media they incorporate both bridging and
bonding techniques in order to maximize the benefits of their social media usage in the form of
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 8
social capital. Essentially, the better well-established an individual’s social capital is the greater
their realm of influence online. However, users must also exercise caution when connecting with
others so that they do not become too vulnerable by over-exposing their personal information.
Quinn (2016) found that there were four valid concerns about sharing personal information
online: information controlcontrolling the amount of information you send out to other people;
power losswhen you share your personal information with others they gain some degree of
power over you; identity lossperceived damage; future life of informationperceived
likelihood of harm. Considering these levels of privacy, it is interesting to see how these privacy
behaviors affect the way users engage with others on social media as well as how those online
behaviors impact the relationships that are formed both on social media and in the physical
world.
Interpersonal Relationships in a Digital Age
Interpersonal relationships are relationships that take place between two or more people
and can include both online (thanks to the Internet) and offline interactions. Although important
and worth the time to explore, the present study does not examine online relationships in depth.
Rather, this study is interested in understanding how individuals use the Internet, in particular
social media, and to what extent it affects their offline interpersonal relationships. Some research
suggests that social media are already changing the way that we interact with each other offline.
Chotpitayasunondh and Douglas (2016) coined a new term known as “phubbing” which
represents “the act of snubbing someone in a social setting by concentrating on one’s phone
instead of talking to the person directly” (p. 10). They found that this “phubbing” behavior was
growing increasingly more commonplace and acceptable and that people were beginning to see
this once-thought-of-as-rude behavior as normal. The extent to which people would phub others
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 9
was directly related to their level of smartphone addiction. As the proclivity of cell phone use
increases, the likelihood of phubbing occurring more frequently will also increase leading to a
more permanent change in the way we interact with each other.
Hertlein (2012) noted that the Internet provides increasingly blurred boundaries between
online and offline relationships. In a study seeking to understand the role of technology in
changing family relationships, Hertlein (2012) found that the rules of interaction with online
peers had several negative effects on daily life such as compromising the function of offline
relationships, detracting from job performance, and increasing the potential for Internet
addictions. Coinciding with these findings, Abbasi and Alghamdi (2017) found that misusing
Facebook can lead to negative societal consequences such as social isolation, distrust in
relationships, infidelity, lack of social cohesion, Facebook addiction, and divorce.
Our online and offline relationships have grown to become so interconnected that what
we do in either of those relationships impacts the other. Kerkhof, Finkenauer, and Muusses
(2011) called this phenomenon a “syntopia” explaining that the physical/social situations and
history of a person influenced what they did and learned online which spilled over into their
offline experiences. Under this lens, Kerkhof et al. (2011) found that those with high compulsive
Internet use experienced decreased quality in their offline relationships, reported decreased
commitment in their relationships, and had more frequent conflicts with their partners.
Conversely, Jenkins-Guarnieri, Wright, and Hudiburgh (2012) found that those with
lower levels of perceived competency at initiating offline relationships was related to increased
use of Facebook. Additionally, heavy social media users have decreased interpersonal
competency at initiating offline relationships meaning that the more a person uses social media
the harder it is for them to initiate new relationships offline. Supporting these findings, Seo,
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 10
Park, Kim, and Park, (2016) revealed that a person who had developed a dependency to their cell
phone experienced decreased attention and increased depression which led to a negative impact
on their social relationships with their friends. Even when people would hide their online
addictions or relationships from their partners they still reported that daily tasks were unfinished
and that levels of sexual intimacy with their primary partner had decreased (Underwood &
Findlay, 2004).
Social media do not only impact our relationships with others, they also impact our
relationship with ourselves and how we perceive the world around us. Kerkhof et al. (2011)
found that compulsive Internet users were lonelier, more depressed, and generally exhibited
poorer social skills than noncompulsive Internet users indicating that these negative
characteristics were brought about by their overuse of the Internet. Additional research revealed
that overuse of social networking sites significantly affects the lives of adolescents with negative
consequences on their personal, psycho-social well-being (Marino, Vieno, Pastore, Albery,
Frings, & Spada, 2016). Finally, Seo, Park, Kim, and Park, (2016) claimed that the more
problematic mobile phone addiction becomes, the more people will experience decreased self-
esteem and emotional well-being.
From the aforementioned research, it is clear that our use of social networking sites
influences our offline relationships and vice-versa. To further explore the depth to which social
networking sites affect our emotions, four psycho-sociological problems will be placed under
scrutiny.
FOMO and Anxiety
Fear of missing out (FOMO) is the psychological mentality that individuals might be
missing out on a social opportunity or situation. This mentality requires that they stay constantly
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 11
connected with others and updated about what their friends are doing (Beyens et al., 2016; Elhai
et al., 2016). The need for these individuals to stay constantly connected with their peers has led
to problematic smartphone use. A side effect to overusing a smartphone is decreased emotional
self-control which is defined by two processes: decreased cognitive reappraisal (inability to
assess your mental or emotional state in a different way) and increased emotional suppression
(suppressing one’s emotions often leads to a buildup of pressure and stress) both of which lead to
an inability to regulate emotions properly (Elhai et al., 2016). Elhai et. al (2016) argued:
Overusing one’s smartphone does not account fully for depression or anxiety; rather,
other intervening variables may play a role. Specifically, less behavioral activation and
(for depression only) more emotion suppression appear to account for this relationship.
Problematic smartphone use may interfere with other pleasurable activities and disrupt
social activities thereby reducing behavioral activation and subsequently increasing
depression. It is possible that emotional suppression, a correlate of problematic use,
disrupts adaptive processing of emotions, which in turn is associated with greater
depression (p. 514).
While depression will be discussed in greater detail later on, this comment suggests that it is not
social media itself that is causing these mental problems, but rather the misuse/overuse/abuse of
it by those who use it.
In a study conducted by Lai, Altavilla, Ronconi, and Aceto (2016), an EEG brain scanner
was used to detect the parts of the brain that were illuminated when the participant was exposed
to certain images. In this study they examined FOMO, social inclusive experiences, and social
exclusive experiences. Their findings showed that those with higher FOMO ratings were more
aware of the state of mind of others involved in positive social interactions and they showed a
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 12
higher need for self-approval which could be the reason why people keep returning to social
media (Lai et al., 2016).
Closely related to FOMO is anxiety which manifests itself frequently in the lives of those
who use social media and experience FOMO. Cheever, Rosen, Carrier, and Chavez (2014)
sought to explore when anxiety manifested itself in the lives of college students who were
separated from their cell phones. After collecting reasons each participant used their cell phone
and acquiring data for how long each participant uses their cell phone for the activities they
mentioned, the researchers found that the average amount of time each college student spent on
their phone each day was 13 hours and the top listed reasons for their use, in order from most
used to least used, were as follows: texting, listening to music, visiting websites, talking on the
phone, using email, watching TV/movies, playing games, and reading books. The amount of
time for each activity was averaged together and divided into three categories of low daily usage
(1-7 hours), moderate daily usage (7.5-16.5 hours), and high daily usage (17-64.5 hours).
The findings of the study revealed that those who were low daily users experienced little
to no anxiety while taking the surveys. Those who were moderate users initially experienced
high anxiety due to the increased length of time to complete the second survey compared to the
time spent to complete the first survey, but the anxiety plateaued during the third survey. For the
high daily usage group there was a significant increase in the length of time spent to complete
each of the three surveys indicating that anxiety increased over time and continued to rise
(Cheever et al., 2014). These findings had less to do with whether or not the person was
separated from their phone and more to do with how heavy of a user they were. This study
highlights that people who use their phones excessively will experience high anxiety when they
are separated from them. This could explain why those who have their cell phones with them and
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 13
use them regularly will experience high rates of anxiety when they are separated from being
active on social media.
Depression and Loneliness
Along with FOMO and anxiety, depression and loneliness contribute to the mental health
problems caused by social media use. One study found that many high school and college
students are dealing with anxiety and depression rates that are five times higher than youth who
were studied during the Great Depression era (AP, 2010). There have been many theories as to
why this might be the case, but several scholars believe that heavy social media use, such as
Facebook and Instagram, may contribute to this growing problem (Tandoc, Ferrucci, & Duffy,
2015). In countries where social media use is high, reports show that loneliness and depression
have increased dramatically within the past decade (Pittman & Reich, 2016). There is no doubt
that increased social media use and higher rates of depression and loneliness are linked.
Tandoc et al. (2015) conducted a study where they found that heavy users of Facebook
experienced higher levels of Facebook envy than light viewers and that they reported feeling
more symptoms of depression. The study also found that heavy Facebook users engaged in
Facebook surveillance, which was akin to lurking, and that this behavior was mediated by envy
(Tandoc et al., 2015). In other words, those who were heavy Facebook users experienced higher
levels of envy and depression which caused them to engage in Facebook surveillance because the
more they saw on Facebook the more they wanted and the worse they felt.
Depression and loneliness go hand in hand with each other, but they are not the same
thing. Pittman and Reich (2016) expounded on the relationship between depression and
loneliness by surveying 253 students asking them about their experiences with image-based
social media platforms (Instagram and Snapchat) vs. text-based social media platforms (Twitter
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 14
and SMS texting). Their findings indicated that solely image-based platforms led to a happier,
more satisfied, less lonely life because images offered the intimacy that a face-to-face
conversation contains allowing the viewer to feel a stronger connection with the situation being
presented in the image. Text-based platforms showed no relationship to increased or decreased
psychological well-being (Pittman et al., 2016). These results might possibly explain one way to
overcome loneliness, however, one can still feel depressed without feeling lonely and vice-versa.
Loneliness is the lack of shared companionship with someone else. Depression is an internal
emotion experienced by oneself. When these two feelings are combined with social media use it
can cause a person to feel socially isolated and paralyzed to the point where they don’t think they
can do anything to get out of their situation.
Rosenthal, Buka, Marshall, Carey, and Clark (2016) conducted a longitudinal study that
examined families and their negative experiences on Facebook. Results indicated that all of the
negative Facebook experiences that were measured were significantly associated with depressive
symptoms (Rosenthal et al., 2016). This study is different from the others previously cited in the
present study because it deals with actual negative experiences causing depressive symptoms as
opposed to users comparing their lives with the good and happy lives they see on social media
and then choosing to feel depressed from it. Either way, the use of Facebook can cause an
increase in feelings of depression and isolation leading to loneliness.
There is a lot of research that has been conducted on how social media use affects the
individual who is using it for good or bad. A lot of research has also been conducted on why
people choose to engage with the types of media that they do. However, there is very little
research on how an individual’s choice to engage with certain types of media influences their
emotions and how those emotions then impact their offline interpersonal relationships. Based on
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 15
this analysis the author posits three research questions and several hypotheses in order to better
understand these phenomena:
Research Questions/Hypotheses
RQ1: How does the use of social media influence the quality of the user’s interpersonal
relationships?
RQ2: How does the use of social media influence the user’s overall emotional well-
being?
RQ3: Why do people use social media?
H1a: Increased time spent on social media will lead to decreased overall quality of the
users’ interpersonal relationships.
H1b: High frequency of accessing social media will lead to decreased overall quality of
the users’ interpersonal relationships.
H2a: Increased time spent on social media will lead to the user’s decreased overall
emotional well-being.
H2b: High frequency of accessing social media will lead to the user’s decreased overall
emotional well-being.
H3a: Emotional well-being mediates the relationship between time spent on social media
and the overall quality of the users’ interpersonal relationships.
H3b: Emotional well-being mediates the relationship between frequency of accessing
social media and the overall quality of the users’ interpersonal relationships.
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 16
Method
This study incorporates a mixed-method approach which includes a survey of
quantitative Likert-like questions and several qualitative short answer questions. The sample for
this study included social media users between 18-62 years of age. A survey was built in
Qualtrics and distributed on Facebook pages, sub-Reddit accounts, and Twitter from 9
February9 March 2018. The sub-Reddit accounts where the survey was posted included:
“SampleSize,” “SaltLakeCity,” “Australia,” “AnythingGoesNews,” and “GradSchool.” All who
desired to participate were invited to do so. The sample size for this study was 750 participants
of whom 627 completed the survey.
The survey is made up of several scales. The first scale was adapted from Olufadi’s
(2016) ‘SONTUS’ which measures time spent on social media and has a Cronbach’s alpha of
.92. The participants answered two questions regarding their social media use: “I use social
media ___ each week (never, once, 2-3 times, 3+ times)” and “Each time I use social media I
typically use it for 0-10 min. 11-30 min. 30+ min. These questions were used in order to
determine if the participant was a high or low social media user as well as how frequently they
accessed social media each day.
The second scale was adapted from Rosenberg (1989) which includes 10 items and is
used to measure the emotional well-being of an individual. This scale was selected because it has
been tested to provide a high reliability and generalizability due to its sample size of over 5,000
participants. This scale is measured on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly agree, 5 = strongly
disagree) and all 10 of the following questions were used with no alterations:
1. I feel that I am a person of worth, at least on an equal plane with others
2. I feel that I have a number of good qualities
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 17
3. All in all, I am inclined to feel that I am a failure
4. I am able to do things as well as most other people
5. I feel I do not have much to be proud of
6. I take a positive attitude toward myself
7. On the whole, I am satisfied with myself
8. I wish I could have more respect for myself
9. I certainly feel useless at times
10. At times I think I am no good at all
The final portion of the survey is built from two separate scales which were combined to
study the quality of the participants interpersonal relationships. The first portion comes from
Hendrick’s (1988) 7-item Interpersonal Relations Scale which was adapted to match the format
of Garthoeffner, Henry, & Robinson’s (1993) 49-item Modified Interpersonal Relationship
Scale. Both scales are measured using a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly agree, 5 = strongly
disagree) and includes the following questions that were originally separated into multiple
categories and have been adapted for the purposes of this study:
Interpersonal Relations Scale (Hendrick, 1988)
1. How well does your partner meet your needs?
2. In general, how satisfied are you with your relationship?
3. How good is your relationship compared to most?
4. How often do you wish you hadn’t gotten into this relationship?
5. To what extent has your relationship met your original expectations?
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 18
6. How much do you love your partner?
7. How many problems are there in your relationship?
Modified Interpersonal Relationship scale (Garthoeffner, Henry, & Robinson, 1993).
Trust
1. There are times when my partner cannot be trusted
2. My partner would tell a lie if he/she could gain by it
3. In our relationship, I have to be alert or my partner is likely to take advantage of me
4. My partner is honest mainly because of a fear of being caught
5. I’m better off if I don’t trust my partner too much
6. Even though my partner provides me with many reports and stories, it is hard to get an
objective account of things
7. There is no simple way to decide if my partner is telling the truth
8. In our relationship, I am occasionally distrustful and expect to be exploited
9. My partner can be counted on to do what he/she says they will do
10. I do not believe my partner would cheat on me even if he/she could get away with it
11. My partner can be relied on to keep his/her promises
12. My partner treats me fairly and justly
13. The advice my partner gives cannot be regarded as being trustworthy
14. I am afraid my partner will hurt my feelings
15. My partner pretends to care about me than he/she really does
16. My partner is likely to say what he/she really believes rather than what he/she thinks I want
to hear
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 19
17. I wonder how much my partner really cares about me
18. I believe most things my partner says
Self-disclosure
1. I can express deep, strong feelings to my partner
2. I feel comfortable expressing almost anything to my partner
3. In our relationship, I feel I am able to expose my weaknesses
4. I do not show deep emotions to my partner
5. I share and discuss my problems with my partner
6. I tell my partner some things of which I am very ashamed
7. It is hard for me to tell my partner about myself
8. I talk with my partner about why certain people dislike me
9. We are very close to each other
10. In our relationship, I’m cautious and play it safe
11. I discuss with my partner the things I worry about when Im with a person of the opposite sex
12. I’m afraid of making mistakes with my partner
13. I touch my partner when I feel warmly toward him/her
14. It’s hard for me to act natural when I’m with my partner
Genuineness
1. My partner really cares about what happens to me
2. It is safe to believe that my partner is interested in my welfare
3. My partner is truly sincere in his/her promises
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 20
4. My partner is sincere and practices what he/she preaches
Empathy
1. My way of doing things is apt to be misunderstood by my partner
2. I feel my partner misinterprets what I say
3. I sometimes stay away from my partner because I fear doing or saying something I might
regret afterwards
4. My partner doesn’t really understand me
5. I sometimes wonder what hidden reason my partner has for doing nice things for me
Comfort
1. I seek my partner’s attention when I’m facing troubles
2. I would like my partner to be with me when Im lonely
3. I feel comfortable when Im alone with my partner
4. I would like my partner to be with me when I receive bad news
5. I feel relaxed when we are together
6. I face life with my partner with confidence
Communication
1. I listen carefully to my partner and help him/her solve problems
2. I understand my partner and sympathize with his/her feelings
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 21
Several of the questions are asked negatively and were reverse-coded during final
analysis of the data. In the survey provided in Appendix A the reverse-coded items are all in
italics.
Also included in this survey are several short answer questions offering additional
insights into why people use social media and what their perceptions are of how social media is
affecting their emotions and their relationships. These short answer questions were developed by
the author and they bring a qualitative element into this study thus forming it into a mixed-
methods study which helps to strengthen it by combining it with quantitative data because a
combination of these two methods is stronger than each one separately (Wisdom & Creswell,
2013). The four short answer questions include:
1. Why do you use social media?
2. Does your use of social media influence your emotions? If so, how?
3. Does your use of social media influence your relationships? If so, how?
4. Explain what you think it would be like to go one week without using your cell phone
or accessing any social networking sites.
The quantitative data were analyzed using the statistical software known as SPSS. The
independent variables are the amount of time spent on social media and the frequency with
which the individual accessed the social networking sites. The dependent variables are the
quality of the interpersonal relationships and the emotional well-being of the individual.
Emotional well-being also served as the meditating variable between the amount of time spent on
social media and the quality of the user’s interpersonal relationships. The qualitative questions
were coded by creating categories based on the answers of the participants and were grouped
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 22
together in a table. To test for reliability, the survey was examined by and distributed to
professors to make sure that the items on the survey worked for the purposes of this study.
Altogether, the survey consists of 73 questions including 4 qualitative short-answer
questions, 64 quantitative Likert scale questions, and 5 demographic questions. The demographic
questions asked about age, gender, ethnicity, romantic relationship status, and romantic
relationship length. The fourth qualitative question “Explain what you think it would be like to
go one week without using your cell phone or accessing any social networking sites” was
omitted during data analysis because it was found to be irrelevant to the direction of the study.
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 23
Results
The sample size for this study was 627. The average age for the participants was 28.36
with 510 participants being between the ages of 18-34 and 117 of the participants being between
the ages of 35-62. There were 373 (59.5%) female participants with 246 (39.2%) male
participants and 8 (1.3%) who preferred not to identify with a gender. The ethnicity of the
participants was as follows: White/Caucasian = 543 (86.6%), Hispanic = 14 (2.2%), Latino = 4
(.6%), African = 7 (1.1%), African American = 7 (1.1%), Asian = 14 (2.2%), Native American =
2 (.3%), Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander = 4 (.6%), European = 15 (2.4%), Other = 17
(2.7%). The status of the participants’ relationships were as follows: single, never married = 198
(31.6%), currently in a relationship = 153 (24.4%), married = 259 (41.3%), divorced = 13
(2.1%), separated = 4 (.6%). Finally, the length of time that participants were in the relationship
they said they were in is as follows: not currently in a relationship = 145 (23.1%), less than one
year = 72 (11.5%), between 1 and 3 years = 117 (18.7%), between 3 and 7 years = 119 (19.0%),
between 7 and 10 years = 52 (8.3%), and more than 10 years = 122 (19.5%). Of this sample, 278
(44.3%) participants indicated that they spend 0-10 min. on social media each time they access it,
with 255 (40.7%) spending 11-30 min. and 94 (15%) spending 30+ min. Lastly, 55 (8.8%)
participants said that they access social media at least one time daily, 131 (20.9%) accessed
social media 2-3 times daily, and 441 (70.3%) access social media 3+ times every day. Those
who said that they never use social media were removed from the study as the author was not
interested in studying those who did not spend time on social media (see Tables 1-7).
Hypothesis 1a stated that increased time spent on social media would lead to decreased
overall quality of the users’ interpersonal relationships. A Spearman’s rho correlational analysis
was conducted to examine the relationship between the amount of time spent on social media
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 24
and the quality of the users interpersonal relationships. The analysis was significant, r(627) = -
.09, p < .05 (see Table 8). Chronbach’s alpha was reported as α = .95. These tests verified that
for participants who spent more time on social media the quality of their interpersonal
relationships decreased, thus H1a was fully supported.
Hypothesis 1b stated that increased frequency of accessing social media would lead to
decreased overall quality of the users’ interpersonal relationships. A Spearman’s rho
correlational analysis was conducted to examine the relationship between how frequently an
individual accessed social media and the quality of their interpersonal relationships. The analysis
was not significant, r(627) = -.04, p > .05 (see Table 8), indicating that how frequently an
individual accessed social media did not impact the quality of their relationships. Thus, H1b was
not supported.
Hypothesis 2a stated that increased time spent on social media would lead to the user’s
decreased overall emotional well-being. A Spearman’s rho correlational analysis was conducted
to examine the relationship between the amount of time spent on social media and the user’s
overall emotional well-being. The analysis was significant, r(627) = -.115, p < .001 (see Table
8), indicating that for those who spent more time on social media their emotional well-being
decreased. In other words, social media contributed to the user experiencing negative emotions
and moods. Thus H2a was fully supported. To further support H2a, a one-way analysis of
variance (ANOVA) was calculated on how much time a user spent on social media and their
emotional well-being. The analysis was significant, F(2, 624) = 6.39, p = .002 (see Table 10).
Participants who spent 30+ min. on social media per session experienced the greatest decrease in
their emotional well-being (M = 3.57, SD = .92) compared to those who spent 11-30 min. (M =
3.77, SD = .84) or those who only spent 0-10 min. (M = 3.91, SD = .73; see Table 9). To verify
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 25
these results, a Bonferroni Post Hoc test was conducted to find the mean difference between
those who spent 0-10 min. on social media and those who spent 30+ min. (M D = .333*, SE =
.096, p = .002; see Table 11). Once again, H2a was fully supported.
Hypothesis 2b stated that high frequency of use on social media would lead to the user’s
decreased overall emotional well-being. A Spearman’s rho correlational analysis was conducted
to examine the relationship between how frequently an individual accessed social media and
their overall emotional well-being. The analysis was not significant, r(627) = -.01, p > .05 (see
Table 8), indicating that how frequently an individual accessed social media did not impact their
emotional well-being. Thus H2b was not supported.
The author also proposed that emotional well-being could serve as a mediator to the
relationship between time spent on social media and relationship quality, as well as mediate the
relationship between the frequency of using social media and relationship quality. Using a
conditional process modeling program called PROCESS (Hayes, 2008, 2013), the author ran a
hierarchical regression analysis to test H3a and H3b, which posited that emotional well-being
could serve as a mediator for the relationship between social media use and relationship quality.
In H3a, the PROCESS tool revealed that the predictortime spent on social mediaand the
outcomerelationship qualitywere mediated by emotional well-being, F(1,625) = 11.80, p =
.0006. Time spent predicted the mediator (emotional well-being) along path A at a significant
value, r = .14, p < .001; b = -.1601, t(625) = -3.44, p = .0006. The mediator then affected the
quality of relationships along path B at a significant value, r = .37, p < .001; b = 2.73, t(624) =
8.52, p < .000. This significance is greater than the original relationship between the time spent
and the quality of the relationships (path C’), r = .08, p < .05; b = -.03, t(624) = -.82, p > .05.
Thus, H3a was fully supported (see Table 12).
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 26
“In H3b, the PROCESS tool revealed that the predictorfrequency of social media
useand the outcomerelationship qualitywas not mediated by emotional well-being, since
path A was not significant, b = -.04, t(625) = -.82, p > .05. Thus, H3b was not supported (see
Table 12).
Based on these results, the author was able to create models to map the regression
between the mediator (emotional well-being) and the independent and dependent variables which
are included below:
Figure 1
Emotional well-being mediates the relationship between time spent on social media and the
overall quality of the users’ interpersonal relationships (supported).
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 27
Figure 2
Emotional well-being mediates the relationship between frequency of accessing social media and
the overall quality of the users interpersonal relationships (not supported).
The three research questions provide the qualitative part of this study and will now be
analyzed. Each of the short answer questions from the survey were coded and categorized based
on a reading of the participant’s responses to each question. Upon completion of the coding,
some categories were grouped together to form a more cohesive understanding of the topic being
discussed.
RQ1 explored the role that social media played in influencing interpersonal relationships.
In the survey, the question was phrased “Does your use of social media influence your
relationships? If so, how?” Of the 750 partially complete surveys, 643 participants answered this
question (see Table 13).
Of the 643 participants who answered this question, 929 responses were recorded due to
the participants’ responses being placed into multiple categories. The most common response
was that social media use did not influence their relationships. There could be several reasons for
this. One possible explanation is that the question was too vague for an adequate response to be
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 28
warranted leading to the response of “no, it does not influence my relationships.” A second
possibility is that those who responded this way were telling the truth and social media does not
influence their relationships. Previous research, and data found in this study indicate that this is
likely not the case, but without the ability to contact the participants and ask them, these results
are inconclusive. Finally, a third possible explanation is simply survey fatigue as this question
was presented at the end of the survey and the participants were likely tired of answering
questions.
The remaining data are more concrete. For the negative results, 380 participants
responded that social media negatively impacted their relationships by affecting various elements
of their lives (see Table 13). The most common response for the negative effects of social media
on relationships is that it distracts the user from engaging in face-to-face interactions with other
people or activities, thus making the user less social offline. The second most popular response
to this research question included that social media use made the user more edgy, irritated,
impatient, jealous, judged, ignored, or wanting to escape in their relationships. The last notable
responses for the impact social media use has on the user’s relationships included that they spent
less quality time with their significant other offline and that they spent more time comparing
their relationship to those they saw online thus resulting in a decreased overall satisfaction in
their own relationship.
The positive results were similar, in that 331 participants responded that social media
played a positive role in their relationships, with the exception of the category “Happier in my
relationships with less time on social media” due to the fact that the relationship was benefitted
by reducing the amount of time spent on social media so as to spend more quality time offline
with their significant others. The top response for this section was that social media allows for
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 29
people to “keep in touch” with their relationships, especially with close friends and family
members. Other top answers for this section include that social media use strengthened their
relationship in some way and they used it to share images, gifs, memes, or videos that they
thought their significant other would appreciate.
RQ2 examined the role that social media played in influencing the emotional well-being
of the user. In the survey, the question was phrased “Does your use of social media influence
your emotions? If so, how? Of the 750 partially complete surveys, 668 participants answered
this question (see Table 14).
Of the 668 participants who answered this question, 1,294 responses were recorded due
to the participants’ responses being placed into multiple categories. The categories of “FOMO,
“Anxiety,” “Depressed,” and “Lonely” were used because they had been shown to be influenced
by the use of social media in the past (Elhai et al., 2016; Cheever et al., 2014; Tandoc, Ferrucci,
& Duffy, 2015; Pittman & Reich, 2016, respectively). “Depressed/sad” was the category with the
most responses of these four (115). 127 participants responded that social media did not
influence their emotions. The majority of the participants experienced negative emotions from
their use of social media with the largest negative category (153) being that social media
influenced them to feel frustrated, annoyed, irritated, distracted, or stressed with 115 of the
participants responding that they felt depressed or sad from their use. Social comparison (107)
was the third most popular response followed by life dissatisfaction (84) and anger (57) or
wasted time (57). In addition to the remaining categories found in Table 14, other emotions were
mentioned throughout the responses that are worth mentioning, however, they were not abundant
enough to merit their own category. These additional negative emotions include: doubt, worry,
and being frightened or scared.
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 30
The happy/positive category holds the highest number (152) of responses for users who
reported receiving benefits from using social media indicating that their use of social media
influenced them to feel happy or positive from their interaction online. Several participants
responded that humor (43) was an affected emotion indicating that their use of social media
caused them to laugh or that the content they viewed was funny to them. Similar to RQ1, a small
number (31) of responses indicated that people were happier when they spent less time on social
media. This study includes a lesser-known term called “Mudita (23),or sympathetic joy, which
is a Buddhist term for finding joy in the happiness and success of others (Salzberg, 1995). This
concept is the opposite of schadenfreude which is the pleasure we feel due to another’s
misfortune. Mudita was selected because there is no word in the English language that
encapsulates the idea of sympathetic joy, and it became a category because several participants
responded that they were happy for their friends or family members when they saw good things
happen to them such as getting married, graduating from school, or starting a new job. For
Mudita to be real, the observer cannot receive any direct benefit from the other person’s success;
the observer is simply happy for the other because of their success with no reward on behalf of
the observer. The common scenario used to describe this idea is the happiness a parent feels for
the success of their child (U Pandita, 2006).
RQ3 was simpler than RQ1 and RQ2 as it merely dealt with reasons why people use
social media. RQ3 was phrased “Why do you use social media?” Of the 750 partially complete
surveys, 680 participants answered this question (see Table 15).
Supporting what has been established in previous research, the top reason as to why
people use social media is to connect with friends and family on a regular basis (461). According
to this data set, the second most common response was to read/watch the news (188), but seeing
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 31
as a large portion of this sample came from Reddit, this is not surprising because Reddit is a
large news aggregate. Other reasons that people use social media are to find fun/entertaining
content (176), learn something new (91), because they are bored (77), to waste time (59), to
create or share content (55), for work/business (48), to escape their offline life or emotions (43),
and to learn about and participate in local events (25).
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 32
Discussion
The primary purpose of this study was to determine if a relationship existed between
excessive social media use and the overall emotional well-being of that individual as well as the
quality of the individual’s interpersonal relationships. A mixed-methods survey was distributed
on Facebook and Reddit and a sample size of 627 participants completed the survey. The
quantitative results were analyzed using SPSS and the qualitative responses were coded into
categories.
The results from H1a revealed that the more time an individual spent on social media the
more the quality of their relationships decreased. This supports findings by Kerkhof et al. (2011)
who also found that those with high compulsive Internet use experienced decreased offline
relationship quality. Results from H1b revealed that the number of times a user accessed social
media, or the frequency, did not play a significant role in altering their relationships. This is
somewhat surprising considering that the majority of the participants (70.3%) responded that
they accessed social media more than three times each day (see Table 2). However, upon further
consideration, these results are akin to picking up the TV remote and idly flipping through the
channels. By spending only a few seconds on each channel the viewer is not affected very
greatly, but if the viewer were to stop and watch one of the shows on TV, the content of that
show would be more influential on the viewer. These findings also support Hertlein (2012) who
found that interaction with online peers contributed to compromising the function of offline
relationships and increased the potential for Internet addictions. To summarize, a person may
access social media several times each day to respond to messages, check notifications, or even
lightly browse their feed, but their frequency of use is not as important as how long they spend
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 33
using social media which serves as a much more influential factor in altering their thoughts that
eventually lead to a change in the way they interact with others.
The results for H2a found that excessive use of social media was correlated with
decreased overall emotional well-being of the user. This supports Seo et al. (2016) who found
that cell phone dependency, and by extension social media usage, led to decreased attention and
increased depression which negatively impacted their social relationships. Cell phone addiction
also contributed to declining self-esteem and emotional well-being. Although counterintuitive,
staying connected to the world through the use of cell phones actually makes us more isolated in
the more important areas of our lives such as spending time face-to-face with those in our close
proximity.
The results found in H2a also support several other studies that have examined various
effects of social media use on emotions. Seo, Park, Kim, and Park, (2016) found that a person
who had developed a dependency to their cell phone experienced decreased attention and
increased depression which led to a negative impact on their social relationships with their
friends. They also claimed that the more problematic smartphone addiction becomes the more
people will experience decreased emotional well-being and increased anxiety (Seo et al., 2016;
Cheever et al., 2014). Kerkhof et al. (2011) found that compulsive Internet users were lonelier,
more depressed, and generally exhibited poorer social skills. Lastly, Tandoc et al. (2015) found
that heavy Facebook users experienced higher levels of envy and depression causing them to
engage in Facebook surveillance because the more they saw on Facebook the more they wanted
and the worse they felt.
The results for H2b revealed that frequency of social media use had the same impact on
emotional well-being as it did on relationships from H1b: none. The frequency of accessing
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 34
social media has little to do with relationships or emotions, but the amount of time spent on
social media serves as the more important variable that has a more direct impact on the quality of
the relationships of the user and their overall emotional well-being. In spite of this, it is possible
that frequently accessing social media, even for short periods of time, disrupt the free flow of
thought that leads to deeper thoughts and personal awareness. One participant remarked on this
thought by saying, “social media takes away from critical time to be alone with oneself and not
being distracted by something. I think social media usage results in a mental health decline not
because it results in a feeling of being left out from seeing what everyone else is up to, but
because people feel the need to be entertained constantly and don't know how to be by
themselves anymore.”
The secondary purpose to this study was to determine if the relationship between time
spent on social media and the quality of the interpersonal relationships could be mediated by the
emotional well-being of the individual using social media. Conclusive results from H3a revealed
that emotional well-being did indeed serve as a mediator for time spent on social media and the
quality of that user’s interpersonal relationships. To summarize, there is a direct inverse link
between time spent on social media and emotional well-being as well as relationship quality
separately. Also, when emotional well-being is introduced as the mediator, the direct relationship
between time and relationship quality disappears and a new link forms between time on social
media and relationship quality through emotional well-being and it turns into an indirect inverse
relationship.
Expressed in simple terms, when a person spends excessive amounts of time on social
media they likely experience decreased emotional well-being, which contributes to them
experiencing decreased quality in their relationships. The results from H3a mean that when a
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 35
person uses social media for long periods of time their emotions are negatively impacted. After
the person is done using social media they carry their negative emotions with them and they play
a role in how the user interacts with others offline. This interaction is not always negative, but
the quality of that interaction is less than it could have been because the negative emotions the
user brought with them from their social media use impacted that interaction. Seo et al. (2016)
found similar results in their study revealing that a person who had developed an addiction to
their cell phone experienced increased depression which led to a negative impact on their social
relationships with friends.
Moving on to the qualitative portion of this study, the three research questions offered an
in-depth look at what takes place within the minds of those who use social media. For RQ1, the
top response for negative results is that social media distracts the users from engaging in
wholesome activities and with people who are offline. This happens frequently when groups of
friends visit a restaurant together and one or all of them spend the majority of their time on their
cell phones without speaking to each other during the course of their visit. Chotpitayasunondh
and Douglas (2016) referred to this phenomenon as phubbing which is where people snub, or
ignore, others by using their phones. The results from RQ1 (see Table 13) support Abbasi and
Alghamdi (2017) who found that misusing Facebook can lead to negative societal consequences
such as social isolation, distrust in relationships, lack of social cohesion, and Facebook addiction.
Further supporting this claim, one of the participants in the present study responded: “I notice a
difference in me and my partner’s behavior when we are on social media for long periods of time
during any given day. We are less patient and a lot more anxious or on edge. We are more likely
to misunderstand each other or start an argument. We tend to be a lot more lazy.
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 36
The third most popular response for RQ1 was that the quality time spent with people
offline was diminished because of their social media usage. This response directly supports H1a
because it shows that people are aware that their social media use plays a role in harming the
quality of their relationships. One participant summarized the entire purpose of this study
perfectly,
When I use it too much I am inclined to think badly of myself and of the world. I am also
more irritable with my children when I am currently using social media and they need
me.
Other notable responses further support these results. 42 participants stated that they
compare their relationship with relationships they see online leading to them feeling more
dissatisfied in their current relationship. This response was also found to be in the top three for
RQ2. One participant hit the nail on the head when they said, “my monster of comparison, when
it was raging, my marriage would really suffer because I would get angry at what we didn’t have
and not be grateful for what we do have. This would make [my husband] feel like a failure and
bad because he couldn’t provide any of it at the time.” This participant felt that her social media
use put a strain on her relationship with her husband, even when he was not using social media,
because she was comparing her life to those she saw online. This is one of the many examples
found in this study that demonstrate this effect.
Other negative effects that participants said resulted from their social media use included
increased distrust towards others, increased isolation, increased emotional detachment, and
increased unrealistic expectations (see Table 13 for full list).
Although the data from RQ1 strongly indicate that social media are a terrible influence in
the lives of users, they are not completely negative. In fact, the most popular reason people use
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 37
social media is to keep in touch with close friends and family members (Pempek, Yermolayeva,
& Calvert, 2009; Subrahmanyam, Reich, Waechter, & Espinoza, 2008; Wang, Tchernev, &
Solloway, 2012) and the responses in the present study support this claim. Participants also
indicated that social media were used to strengthen their relationships with people they were
close to through connecting and chatting with them, sharing memes and gifs, and receiving
updates on their lives that allow for opportunities to bond. Several participants even said that
social media help them to form new relationships that they never would have otherwise.
This balance between the positive and negative results from social media use is hard to
maintain and the data from RQ1 and H1a seem to push toward the negative. In fact, 20
participants who responded to RQ1 and 31 participants who responded to RQ2 said that they
believed they were happier in their relationships and emotions when they decreased their social
media use indicating that it was their social media use that was causing some degree of strain on
their relationships. One participant stated,
I used to spend a lot more time on social media, but I found that it made me unhappy. I
became jealous of others and their success. I began to compare myself to them, and
thought less of myself. One day a few months ago, I got sick of it, and I deleted
everything besides Facebook. Since then, I have been way happier, and I have grown
to see myself in a much better way. I am also happier for those around me.
The data from RQ2 indicate that social media has a widely negative impact on the
emotions of those who use it with a small positive influence. The four original emotions
discussed earlier (FOMO, depression, loneliness, anxiety) will be examined first. Of these four,
depression scored the highest with 115 respondents saying that their social media influenced
them to feel depressed/sad for a variety of reasons. Combining this finding with the 43
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 38
participants who mentioned envy/jealousy was a common emotion for them offers support to
Tandoc et al. (2015) who said that heavy social media users experienced higher levels of envy
and reported feeling more symptoms of depression.
FOMO placed second among these original four, however it placed 9
th
overall among the
categories of negative emotions. One of the main drawbacks to FOMO is that it motivates the
individual to engage in social surveillance where they are constantly worrying about what
everyone else is doing and whether or not they are being included. This behavior encourages the
individual to stay on social media for longer periods of time so that they dont miss out on any
opportunities and they return to social media more frequently in order to satisfy their higher need
for self-approval (Lai et al., 2016). One participant supported this finding by saying, “I have
found that using social media makes me hyper-aware of others, and more easily able to fall into
tribalistic thinking. It becomes a lot easier to get fired up about something and say ‘oh that
person is such an idiot,’ compared to if I was talking to someone in person.
Loneliness came third with people saying their social media use made them feel
disconnected from their peers even though they were connected online. One participant stated,
“Often times, social media can make me feel more isolated from the people I follow, and the
people around me. I can also leave social media sites feeling more self-conscious and
depressed. There is so much depression, anxiety, and loneliness that seem to be growing, but
these are often related to trivial things such as having a Netflix series end. There is little doubt
that these emotions are becoming more frequent, but are they perhaps related to more shallow
problems instead of real issues?
Anxiety came last out of the four original emotions. Several people responded that they
felt anxiety in connection with other emotions and that they felt it most strongly when they were
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 39
separated from either their cell phones or social media (Cheever et al., 2014). One participant
said, “sometimes, I'll see a cute couple or something and I'll get anxious because I'm so lonely.”
Another participant stated, “it makes me anxious when there’s a lot of bad news.” A third
participant remarked, “sometimes I feel anxious because I wonder why I haven’t been able to
secure a marriage relationship at this point in my life.” These people have realized that some of
their experiences online are impacting their personal lives to the point where they are no longer
satisfied with how their life is going.
A significant number of participants responded that their social media use led to greater
life dissatisfaction. This is closely related to social comparison because the act of comparing
your own life to that of someone else causes you to reflect on the quality of your life and the
majority of people will view their own life as lesser, especially because they are comparing their
worst parts to the best parts of others lives. Social comparison was one of the repeating
offenders for RQ1 and RQ2. Several participants of this study responded that they engaged in
social comparison by comparing their lives to those of their peers. A common theme found
among these participants was that they experienced feelings that their lives were dull, plain,
boring, and bland. They felt that their lives were not satisfying or fulfilling enough leading them
to grow ungrateful for what they did have. One participant wrote, “I find that the more time I
spend on social media the less grateful and satisfied with my life I become. I compare my life
more to the lives I see online and that makes me feel like my life is not very exciting.” Another
respondent stated, “the more time I spend on social media, the less satisfied I am with myself.
In the present study there are elements of social comparison and validation from others
that puts their happiness in the hands of their peers. If their peers do not validate their behaviors
or feelings it will cause a negative effect that can be detrimental to the mental health of the
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 40
individual. With this in mind, it is very possible that people would avoid sharing personal
information such as trials, personal struggles, and the mundane parts of their lives and only share
that which is positive, happy, and exciting thus creating the false notion that all is well and good
in their lives. When other users see this false notion (not that the happy events are false, but that
the image portrayed symbolizes that there are no negative events) they will often compare their
life to the life they see online and feel that their life is dull, boring and depressing. One
participant confirmed these findings this way, “I saw a comment once that when you logged onto
Facebook you were getting the highlight reel of others' lives, and so then -- especially if you
were already depressed -- you'd go away and compare the worst of your life to the best of
others.
Other noteworthy negative results with a quote from a participant for each were anger
(“I’m angry when I compare myself to other people because I feel like I will never measure up”),
time wasting (“sometimes I compare myself to other people, or I will unconsciously check FB
and be upset with myself for being so automatic about it and wasting time”), apathy (“positively,
it provides escapism, something that is good for people in moderation. Negatively, it provides
too much escapism”), and distorted perception of reality (“scrolling through Instagram tends to
encourage my belief that I need to have all of the greatest, newest, best version of everything. I
am aware though that you never know the truth behind a post... but it doesn't change the way that
it makes me feel from time to time”). Relating these findings to H2a, the more time an individual
spends on social media the more likely chance that their emotions, and by extension their
emotional well-being will be negatively impacted.
Each of these negative emotions do not jeopardize the overall emotional well-being of an
individual, however the slow culmination of repeated effects can eventually break down the
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 41
emotional barriers of the social media user causing short-term immediate damage and lasting
damage over time. Once these emotional barriers are suppressed it leaves the door wide open for
additional negative emotions to enter where they were not invited. Repeated exposure can drive
those emotions deeper and deeper until they are very difficult to remove or overcome. However,
like any injury, it may hurt to remove the source of the pain, but it will eventually heal and
hopefully serve as a strength that will never be bothered again.
Why did some of the participants respond the way they did? There are potentially several
reasons explaining why participants responded in this particular way: the participants actually
feel the emotions they are describing, the participants think that they should feel a certain way
based on the content so they then feel that way, or because the participants are choosing to allow
the media to influence them. One participant put it this way, “to allow social media to influence
you is silly. I am a person who knows I am in control of my emotions, if I see something that
affects my emotions on social media I ask myself why am I feeling this and figure out the deeper
reason to those emotions and work it out.” This participant understands that they are in control of
choosing how social media affect them and they actively choose to control their emotions. One
possible explanation for this is the age of the user. As we get older our perception of others
around us changes and we have the tendency to care less about what others think about us. We
grow more confident in who we are. However, many other users do not have this self-awareness
and they are simply passive users who continually choose to allow social media to bring them
down.
As with RQ1, RQ2 shows that social media are a two-sided coin with one side being
negative and the other positive. At least 152 participants responded that their social media use
made them feel happy or positive in some manner. This included seeing happy posts from
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 42
friends, watching uplifting or inspiring videos, receiving positive validation through the form of
likes and supportive comments, and being entertained. These results contradict research from
Hayes, Carr, and Wohn (2016) who explored the meaning that “liking” a post on Facebook (or a
“favorite” on Twitter, etc…) had for both the original poster and the one who “liked” the post.
The results from that study indicated that people devalued Facebook “likes” owing to the fact
that they were more reactionary than conscious. As was shown in the findings in the present
study, this was not the case.
Mudita was the category that surfaced as the most surprising category because it is
completely selfless and deals with an individual being happy for someone else’s success with no
return reward. Overwhelmingly, the qualitative findings of the present study have indicated that
people use social media for themselves. They access social media and interact with people and
content in a very hedonistic fashion seeking for the best benefit, while minimizing the most pain,
from their use. Mudita, however, reveals that some social media users are genuinely happy for
others and that they are not so focused on themselves that they cannot think of others. One
participant summarized their feelings on this idea, “when I read or see the positives in some
peoples lives it gives me absolute joy.
The theory used for the present study was uses and gratifications. RQ3 is a direct
response to this theory by providing answers as to why people use social media. As has already
been shown in previous answers, the top reason people use social media is to connect with
friends and family members (Oeldorf-Hirsch & Sundar, 2016; Wang, Tchernev, & Solloway,
2012). Since this reason repeats itself so frequently, it suggests that people generally intend to
use social media for the purpose of connecting with the people they know as opposed to using it
for alternative purposes. Though this may be true, it brings to light another element of U&G that
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 43
is not commonly discussed: its dark side. A pattern that surfaced from the data was that people
waste time on social media. For those who responded to RQ3, they waste time intentionally, thus
satisfying their use. However, for those who answered RQ2, many responded that they did not
like wasting time online because it made them feel unproductive or lazy. U&G would explain
why we choose the media we do, but its dark side would explain that the side effects of our use
often create deeper holes within us that our intended use did not gratify. From this we can enter a
cyclical pattern of using media to satisfy our needs, having some of our needs gratified and
others enlarged, causing us to return to social media to satisfy the new needs, thus becoming
caught in a never-ending process potentially leading to addiction. One participant highlighted
these conclusions with this statement,
[Social media] is the only way I have to stay in contact with some people, but I have
noticed that the more I use social media the more I feel depressed and compare myself to
others. I try to cut out or reduce social media but it is an addiction.
This participant said that the reason they use social media is to connect with family and friends.
However, while they are doing that they are being exposed to additional content they probably
were not intending to see which causes them to make comparisons and feel depressed. They then
said that they have tried to quit, but it has become an addiction, suggesting that they will likely
return to social media to attempt to gratify their new needs of feeling depressed and invalidated.
Adam Alter (2017), in his recent book Irresistible, defined behavioral addiction as
something you keep doing even though you hate yourself for doing it. He further explains that
addictions rise slowly over time and are often unnoticeable until they are well set. The findings
in the present study indicate that there are potentially strong elements of addiction resulting from
excessive social media use. Two previous studies have highlighted that addictive behaviors exist
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 44
as a result of social media use, but they have not explored them in detail (Seo et al., 2016; Abbasi
et al., 2017). The present study does not insinuate that all social media users are addicts.
However, the findings presented here strongly suggest that there is a growing problem from
excessive social media use.
To summarize the findings, excessive social media use leads to decreased emotional
well-being by triggering an increased response to negative emotions such as FOMO, depression,
loneliness, anxiety, social comparison, decreased life satisfaction, anger, wasting time,
frustration, increased isolation and a slew of others. Overindulging in social media also leads to
decreased relationship quality with others, including close relationships, by increasing the
potential for distrust, relationship dissatisfaction, emotional detachment, isolation, fewer offline
interactions, a skewed perception of reality, and in rare cases the ending of a relationship. These
negative emotions seem to indicate that addictive behaviors are on the rise and are closely related
to overuse of social media.
On the other side of the coin, social media use also produces positive effects on
emotional well-being such as happiness, Mudita, humor, support, validation, and a more frequent
connection with loving friends and family members. Our use of social media can also produce
positive effects on relationships by allowing to people to keep in touch with each other more
frequently, sharing uplifting and inspirational content with others, receiving validation for
personal achievement, and even forming new relationships.
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 45
Conclusion
This study has dealt with how the use of social media impacts the emotional well-being
of the user and their offline interpersonal relationships. As social beings, it is natural for us to
want to connect with those around us and social media has certainly made it easier to do so.
However, as has been shown in this study, social media is not without its pitfalls. Backed by
quantitative data, the qualitative findings in this study provide compelling evidence that social
media use offers more negative consequences than benefits. The data imply that unless some
appropriate actions are taken to be more aware of how our social media use impacts us and what
we can do to resolve those issues then we will likely develop negative habits and further plunge
ourselves into a state of deeper emotional distress by passively allowing our health and
relationships to deteriorate.
This study confirms prior research on uses and gratifications theory, social media effects,
and relationship problems, but also offers additional data that perhaps takes past research beyond
previous limits to showcase that social media play a role in creating addictive behaviors. It is
highly possible that social media are creating a society of addicts. The question that each social
media needs to ask themselves is how do we know if our social media use is too much, or if it
has become an addiction for us? This may require taking a break from social media for a short
while and taking inventory of our lives in order to better see what needs social media has been
gratifying and then to make the choice of either continuing to use social media to fill those needs
to or to find alternative sources of gratification that are more lasting.
Future research should examine how addictive behaviors are created or strengthened
through excessive social media use. Future research should also examine what the ideal amount
of time spent on social media should be. Is there an appropriate balance between too much social
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 46
media use and none at all? If there is, what is it and does moderating how much time we spend
on social media influence whether or not it will turn into an addiction? Another idea for future
research is to examine gender differences for social media usage. This study did not explore the
differences between male and female social media use and it might be interesting to see if the
emotions expressed in the qualitative data are more a result of the 60% female responses or if
they accurately reflect the larger population. This can be done by studying only male or only
female participants instead of grouping them together as this study has done.
One of the possible limitations to collecting richer data than was acquired in this study
was that the short answer questions in the survey were too vague. Having such non-specific
questions resulted in somewhat vague answers. However, the data that were collected still served
to be beneficial in many ways. Another limitation was that all of the participants were recruited
only through social media which is also where they completed the survey. This method could
have caused the results to become negligible due to trolling and personal bias, however, the
author feels compelled to justify that the data collected here are legitimate and not merely a
byproduct of being trolled by the research participants. As evidenced by two groundbreaking
books on trolling behavior by Coleman (2015) and Phillips (2016), trolls are often inclined to
share their authentic thoughts on the topic with researchers becausein addition to participating
in the behaviorthey find the topic fascinating to discuss with dispassionate observers.
One final thought for this research study is that social media are still relatively new to the
world and as this phenomenon continues to develop, only continued research and time will be
able to identify what is truly happening to mankind in the years to come.
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 47
References
Abbasi, I. S., & Alghamdi, N. G. (2017). When flirting turns into infidelity: The Facebook
dilemma. The American Journal of Family Therapy, 45(1), 1-14.
doi:10.1080/01926187.2016.1277804
Alter, A. (2017). Irresistible. Penguin Press: New York.
Alajmi, M. A., Alharbi, A. H., & Ghuloum, H. F. (2016). Predicting the use of Twitter in
developing countries: Integrating innovation attributes, uses and gratifications, and trust
approaches. Informing Science: The International Journal of an Emerging
Transdiscipline, 19, 215-237. doi:10.28945/3534
AP. (2010). Study: Students more stressed now than during Depression? USA Today. Retrieved
from: http://usatoday30.usatoday.com/news/education/2010-01-12-students-depression-
anxiety_N.htm
Beyens, I., Frison, E., & Eggermont, S. (2016). “I don’t want to miss a thing”: Adolescents’ fear
of missing out and its relationship to adolescents’ social needs, Facebook use, and
Facebook related stress. Computers in Human Behavior, 64, 1-8.
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.083
Blumler, J. G., & Katz, E. (1974). The uses of mass communications: Current perspectives on
gratifications research. Sage Publications.
Bourdieu, P., & Wacquant, L. (1992). An Invitation to Reflexive Sociology. Chicago: University
of Chicago Press.
Cheever, N. A., Rosen, L. D., Carrier, L. M., & Chavez, A. (2014). Out of sight is not out of
mind: The impact of restricting wireless mobile device use on anxiety levels among low,
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 48
moderate and high users. Computers in Human Behavior, 37, 290-297.
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.002
Cheung, C. M. K., Chiu, P., & Lee, M. K. O. (2011). Online social networks: Why do students
use Facebook? Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 1337-1343.
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2010.07.028
Chotpitayasunondh, V. & Douglas, K. M. (2016). How “phubbing” becomes the norm: The
antecedents and consequences of snubbing via smartphone. Computers in Human
Behavior, 63, 9-18. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.018
Coleman, G. (2014). Hacker, hoaxer, whistleblower, spy: The many faces of Anonymous.
London and New York: Verso Books.
Elhai, J. D., Levine, J. C., Dvorak, R. D., & Hall, B. J. (2016). Fear of missing out, need for
touch, anxiety and depression are related to problematic smartphone use. Computers in
Human Behavior, 63, 509-516. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.079
Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of Facebook “friends:” Social
capital and college students’ use of online social network sites. Journal of Computer-
Mediated Communication, 12, 1143-1168. doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00367.x
Gan, C. (2018). Gratifications for using social media. Information Development, 34(2), 139-147.
doi:10.1177/0266666916679717
Garthoeffner, J. L., Henry, C. S., & Robinson, L. (1993). The modified interpersonal relationship
scale: Reliability and validity. Psychological Reports, 73, 995-1004.
doi:10.2466/pr0.1993.73.3.995
Hayes, A. F. (2008). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A
regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 49
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Model templates for PROCESS for SPSS and SAS. Retrieved from
http://www.afhayes.com/public/templates.pdf.
Hayes, R. A., Carr, C. T., & Wohn, D. Y. (2016). One click, many meanings: Interpreting
paralinguistic digital affordances in social media. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic
Media, 60(1), 171-187. doi:10.1080/08838151.2015.1127248
Hendrick, S. S. (1988). A generic measure of relationship satisfaction. Journal of Marriage and
the Family, 50(1), 93–98.
Hertlein, K. M. (2012). Digital dwelling: Technology in couple and family relationships.
Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 61(3), 374-387. doi:10.1111/j.1741-
3729.2012.00702.x
Jenkins-Guarnieri, M. A., Wright, S. L., & Hudiburgh, L. M. (2012). The relationships among
attachment style, personality traits, interpersonal competency, and Facebook use. Journal
of Applied Developmental Psychology, 33(6), 294-301.
doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2012.08.001
Katz, E., Blumler, J. G., & Gurevitch, M. (1973). Uses and gratifications research. Public
Opinion Quarterly, 37(4), 509-523. Retrieved from
http://poq.oxfordjournals.org.erl.lib.byu.edu/
Kerkhof, P., Finkenauer, C., & Muusses, L. D. (2011). Relational consequences of compulsive
Internet use: A longitudinal study among newlyweds. Human Communication Research,
37(2), 147-173. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2010.01397.x
Kross, E., Verduyn, P., Demiralp, E., Park, J., Lee, D. S., Lin, N., & Ybarra, O. (2013).
Facebook use predicts declines in subjective well-being in young adults. Public Library
of Science, 1(8), e69841.
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 50
Lai, C. Altavilla, D. Ronconi, A., & Aceto, P. (2016). Fear of missing out (FOMO) is associated
with activation of the right middle temporal gyrus during inclusion social cue. Computers
in Human Behavior, 61, 516-521. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.072
Levy, M. R., & Windahl, S. (1984). Audience activity and gratifications: A conceptual
clarification and exploration. Communication Research, 11, 5178.
doi:10.1177/009365084011001003
Marino, C., Vieno, A., Pastore, M., Albery, I. P., Frings, D., & Spada, M. M. (2016). Modeling
the contribution of personality, social identity and social norms to problematic Facebook
use in adolescents. Addictive Behaviors, 63, 51-56. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.07.001
Nezlek, J. B., Richardson, D. S., Green, L. R., & Schatten-Jones, E. C. (2002). Psychological
well-being and day-to-day social interaction among older adults. Personal Relationships,
9(1), 57-71. Retrieved from http://www.wiley.com.erl.lib.byu.edu/WileyCDA/
Oeldorf-Hirsch, A., & Sundar, S. S. (2016). Social and technological motivations for online
photo sharing. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 60(4), 624-642.
doi:10.1080/08838151.2016.1234478
Olufadi, Y. (2016). Social networking time use scale (SONTUS): A new instrument for
measuring the time spent on the social networking sites. Telematics and Informatics, 33,
452-471. doi:10.1016/j.tele.2015.11.002
Palmgreen, P., Wenner, L. A., & Rayburn, J. D. (1980). Relations between gratifications sought
and obtained: A study of television news. Communication Research, 7, 161–192.
doi:10.1177/009365028000700202
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 51
Park, N., Kee, K. F., & Valenzuela, S. (2009). Being immersed in social networking
environment: Facebook groups, uses and gratifications, and social outcomes.
Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 12, 729733. doi:10.1089/cpb.2009.0003
Pempek, T. A., Yermolayeva, Y. A., & Calvert, S. L. (2009). College students’ social
networking experiences on Facebook. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30,
227-238. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.010
Petersen, C., & Johnston, K. A. (2015). The impact of social media usage on the cognitive social
capital of university students. Informing Science: The International Journal of an
Emerging Transdiscipline, 18, 1-30. doi:10.28945/2160
Pew Research Center. (2014). Cell phone and smartphone ownership demographics. Retrieved
from http://www.pewinternet.org/data-trend/mobile/cell-phone-and-
smartphoneownershipdemographics/
Phillips, W. (2015). This is why we can't have nice things: Mapping the relationship between
online trolling and mainstream culture. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Pittman, M., & Reich, B. (2016). Social media and loneliness: Why an Instagram picture may be
worth more than a thousand Twitter words. Computers in Human Behavior, 62, 155-167.
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.084
Putnam, R. D. (2000). Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American community. New
York: Simon & Schuster.
Quan-Haase, A., & Young, A. L. (2010). Uses and gratifications of social media: A comparison
of Facebook and instant messaging. Bulletin of Science, Technology & Society, 30, 350
361. doi:10.1177/0270467610380009
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 52
Quinn, K. (2016). Why we share: A uses and gratifications approach to privacy regulation in
social media use. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 60(1), 61-86.
doi:10.1080/08838151.2015.1127245
Rosen, L. D., Whaling, K., Carrier, L. M., Cheever, N. A., & Rokkum, J. (2013a). The
media/technology usage, attitudes and anxiety scale: An empirical investigation.
Computers in Human Behavior, 29(6), 25012511. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2013.06.006
Rosen, L. D., Whaling, K., Rab, S. A., Carrier, L. M., & Cheever, N. A. (2013b). Is Facebook
creatingiDisorders’’? The link between clinical symptoms of psychiatric disorders and
technology use, attitudes and anxiety. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(3), 1243–1254.
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2012.11.012
Rosenberg, M. (1989). Society and the Adolescent Self-Image (Rev. ed.). Middletown, CT:
Wesleyan University Press.
Rosenthal, S. R., Buka, S. L., Marshall, B. D. L., Carey, K. B., & Clark, M. A. (2016). Negative
experiences on Facebook and depressive symptoms among young adults. Journal of
Adolescent Health, 59, 510-516. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2016.06.023
Rubin, A. M. (2009). Media uses and effects: A uses-and-gratifications perspective on media
effects. In J. Bryant & M. B. Oliver (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and
research, (3rd Ed.), (pp. 165184). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Ryan, T., Chester, A., Reece, J., & Xenos, S. (2014). The uses and abuses of Facebook: A review
of Facebook addiction. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 3, 133–148.
Salzberg, S. (1995). Loving-kindness: The revolutionary art of happiness. Shambhala
Publications.
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 53
Seo, D. G., Park, Y., Kim, M. K., & Park, J. (2016). Mobile phone dependency and its impacts
on adolescents’ social and academic behaviors. Computers in Human Behavior, 63, 282-
292. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.026
Steers, M. N. (2016). ‘It’s complicated’: Facebook’s relationship with the need to belong and
depression. Current Opinion in Psychology, 9, 22-26. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.007
Subrahmanyam, K., Reich, S. M., Waechter, N., & Espinoza, G. (2008). Online and offline
social networks: Use of social networking sites by emerging adults. Journal of Applied
Developmental Psychology, 29, 420-433. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2008.07.003
Tandoc, E. C. J., Ferrucci, P., & Duffy, M. (2015). Facebook use, envy, and depression among
college students: Is facebooking depressing? Computers in Human Behavior, 43, 139-
146. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.10.053
U Pandita, S. (2006). The state of mind called beautiful. Simon and Schuster.
Underwood, H. & Findlay, B. (2004). Internet relationships and their impact on primary
relationships. Behavior Change, 21(2), 127-140.
Wang, Z., Tchernev, J. M., & Solloway, T. (2012). A dynamic longitudinal examination of social
media use, needs, and gratifications among college students. Computers in Human
Behavior, 28, 1829-1839. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2012.05.001
Wisdom, J., & Creswell, J. W. (2013). Mixed methods: Integrating quantitative and qualitative
data collection and analysis while studying patient-centered medical home models.
Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality. Retrieved from:
https://pcmh.ahrq.gov/page/mixed-methods-integrating-quantitative-and-qualitative-data-
collection-and-analysis-while
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 54
Appendix A—Social Media Survey
Time Spent on Social Media (Social networking time use scale ‘SONTUS’)
Olufadi, Y. (2016).
Please indicate how many times, on average, you use social networking sites such as Facebook,
Instagram, Twitter, LinkedIn, Reddit, YouTube, Pinterest, Snapchat, etc., during a typical day:
1. I use social media ____ each day.
Never Once 2-3 times 3+ times
Please indicate how much time you spend using social networking sites such as Facebook,
Instagram, Twitter, LinkedIn, Reddit, YouTube, Pinterest, Snapchat, etc., during each session:
1. Each time I use social media I typically use it for:
0-10 min. 10-30 min. 30+ min.
1. Why do you use social media?
Self Esteem Scale
The following questions pertain to the emotional perception you have of yourself.
Please indicate the degree to which each statement applies to you by marking one of the
following: strongly disagree (1), somewhat disagree (2), undecided (3), somewhat agree (4),
strongly agree (5). (Rosenberg, 1989)
1. I feel that I am a person of worth, at least on an equal plane with others
2. I feel that I have a number of good qualities
3. All in all, I am inclined to feel that I am a failure
4. I am able to do things as well as most other people
5. I feel I do not have much to be proud of
6. I take a positive attitude toward myself
7. On the whole, I am satisfied with myself
8. I wish I could have more respect for myself
9. I certainly feel useless at times
10. At times I think I am no good at all
2. Does your use of social media influence your emotions? If so, how?
Interpersonal Relationship Scale
Please answer the following questions about your relationship with the person you are closest to
(e.g. family member, spouse, best friend, intimate partner, coworker, etc…). It does NOT need to
be a romantic relationship, although it is all right if it is.
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 55
Please indicate the degree to which each statement applies to you by marking one of the
following: strongly disagree (1), somewhat disagree (2), undecided (3), somewhat agree (4),
strongly agree (5). (Hendrick, 1988; Garthoeffner, Henry, & Robinson, 1993).
1. My close friend/partner meets my needs
2. I am very satisfied with the relationship I have with my close friend/partner
3. Compared to most, my relationship is excellent
4. I often wish I hadn’t gotten into this relationship
5. My relationship has fully met my original expectations
6. My relationship is full of problems
7. There are times when my close friend/partner cannot be trusted
8. My close friend/partner would tell a lie if he/she could gain by it
9. In our relationship, I have to be alert or my close friend/partner is likely to take advantage
of me
10. My close friend/partner is honest mainly because of a fear of being caught
11. I’m better off if I don’t trust my close friend/partner too much
12. Even though my close friend/partner provides me with many reports and stories, it is hard
to get an objective account of things
13. There is no simple way to decide if my close friend/partner is telling the truth
14. In our relationship, I am occasionally distrustful and expect to be exploited
15. My close friend/partner can be counted on to do what he/she says they will do
16. My close friend/partner can be relied on to keep his/her promises
17. My close friend/partner treats me fairly and justly
18. The advice my close friend/partner gives cannot be regarded as being trustworthy
19. I am afraid my close friend/partner will hurt my feelings
20. My close friend/partner pretends to care about me more than he/she really does
21. My close friend/partner is likely to say what he/she really believes rather than what
he/she thinks I want to hear
22. I wonder how much my close friend/partner really cares about me
23. I believe most things my close friend/partner says
24. I can express deep, strong feelings to my close friend/partner
25. I feel comfortable expressing almost anything to my close friend/partner
26. In our relationship, I feel I am able to expose my weaknesses
27. I do not show deep emotions to my close friend/partner
28. I share and discuss my problems with my close friend/partner
29. I tell my close friend/partner some things of which I am very ashamed
30. It is hard for me to tell my close friend/partner about myself
31. I talk with my close friend/partner about why certain people dislike me
32. We are very close to each other
33. In our relationship, I’m cautious and play it safe
34. Im afraid of making mistakes with my close friend/partner
35. It’s hard for me to act natural when I’m with my close friend/partner
36. My close friend/partner really cares about what happens to me
37. It is safe to believe that my close friend/partner is interested in my welfare
38. My close friend/partner is truly sincere in his/her promises
39. My close friend/partner is sincere and practices what he/she preaches
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 56
40. My way of doing things is apt to be misunderstood by my close friend/partner
41. I feel my close friend/partner misinterprets what I say
42. I sometimes stay away from my close friend/partner because I fear doing or saying
something I might regret afterwards
43. My close friend/partner doesn’t really understand me
44. I sometimes wonder what hidden reason my close friend/partner has for doing nice things
for me
45. I seek my close friend/partner’s attention when I’m facing troubles
46. I would like my close friend/partner to be with me when I’m lonely
47. I feel comfortable when I’m alone with my close friend/partner
48. I would like my close friend/partner to be with me when I receive bad news
49. I feel relaxed when we are together
50. I face life with my close friend/partner with confidence
51. I listen carefully to my close friend/partner and help him/her solve problems
52. I understand my close friend/partner and sympathize with his/her feelings
3. Does your use of social media influence your relationships? If so, how?
4. Explain what you think it would be like to go one week without using your cell phone or
accessing any social networking sites.
Demographics
1. As of your most recent birthday, what is your age?
2. What is your gender?
a. Male
b. Female
c. Other
d. Prefer not to answer
3. What is your ethnicity?
a. White/Caucasian
b. Hispanic
c. Latino
d. African
e. African American
f. Asian
g. Native American
h. Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander
i. European
j. Other (please specify)
4. What is your current relationship status?
a. Single, never married
b. In a relationship
c. Married
d. Divorced
e. Widowed
f. Separated
g. Other (please specify)
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 57
5. How long have you been in the relationship you selected in question 4?
a. Not currently in a relationship
b. Less than one year
c. Between 1 and 3 years
d. Between 3 and 7 years
e. Between 7 and 10 years
f. More than 10 years
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 58
Appendix BTables
Table 1
Time
Percent
0-10 min.
44.3
11-30 min.
40.7
30+ min.
15.0
Total
100.0
N = 627
Table 2
Frequency
Percent
Once
8.8
2-3 times
20.9
3+ times
70.3
Total
100.0
N = 627
Table 3
Age
Age
Frequency
Percent
18
31
4.9
19
34
5.4
20
26
4.1
21
50
8.0
22
41
6.5
23
41
6.5
24
33
5.3
25
32
5.1
26
24
3.8
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 59
27
54
8.6
28
54
8.6
29
25
4.0
30
19
3.0
31
11
1.8
32
13
2.1
33
10
1.6
34
12
1.9
35
8
1.3
36
7
1.1
37
7
1.1
38
8
1.3
39
6
1.0
40
6
1.0
41
4
.6
42
8
1.3
43
3
.5
44
2
.3
45
1
.2
46
3
.5
47
3
.5
48
5
.8
49
2
.3
51
6
1.0
52
7
1.1
53
2
.3
54
3
.5
55
2
.3
56
3
.5
57
4
.6
58
2
.3
59
3
.5
60
2
.3
61
2
.3
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 60
62
1
.2
Total
620
98.9
Missing
7
1.1
N = 620
Table 4
Gender
Percent
Male
39.2
Female
59.5
Other
1.3
Total
100.0
N = 627
Table 5
Ethnicity
Percent
White/Caucasian
86.6
Hispanic
2.2
Latino
.6
African
1.1
African American
1.1
Asian
2.2
Native American
.3
Native Hawaiian or Pacific
Islander
.6
European
2.7
Other
2.7
Total
100.0
N = 627
Table 6
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 61
Relationship Status
Frequency
Percent
Single, never married
198
31.6
In a relationship
153
24.4
Married
259
41.3
Divorced
13
2.1
Separated
4
.6
Total
627
100.0
N = 627
Table 7
Relationship Length
Frequency
Percent
Not currently in a relationship
145
23.1
Less than one year
72
11.5
Between 1 and 3 years
117
18.7
Between 3 and 7 years
119
19.0
Between 7 and 10 years
52
8.3
More than 10 years
122
19.5
Total
627
100.0
N = 627
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 62
Table 8
Relationship between dependent variables (relationship quality, emotional well-being) and independent
variables (time, frequency)
Time
Quality
Frequency
Emotions
Spearman’s rho
Time
Correlation
Coefficient
1.000
-.093*
.059
-.115**
Sig. (2-tailed)
-
.020
.140
.004
N
627
627
627
627
Relationship
Quality
Correlation
Coefficient
-.093*
1.000
-.040
.435**
Sig. (2-tailed)
.020
-
.320
.000
N
627
627
627
627
Frequency
Correlation
Coefficient
.059
-.040
1.000
-.011
Sig. (2-tailed)
.140
.320
-
.781
N
627
627
627
627
Emotional
well-being
Correlation
Coefficient
-.115**
.435**
-.011
1.000
Sig. (2-tailed)
.004
.000
.781
-
N
627
627
627
627
N = 627; *p = .05; **p = .01
Table 9
Descriptive Statistics for Time spent on Social
Media and Emotional Well-being
N
Mean
Std. Deviation
0-10 min.
278
3.91
.73
11-30 min.
255
3.77
.84
30+ min.
94
3.57
.92
Total
627
3.80
.81
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 63
N = 627
Table 10
One-way ANOVA for Time spent on
Social Media and Emotional Well-being
df
F
Sig.
Between Groups
2
6.387
.002
Within Groups
624
Total
626
N = 627
Table 11
Post Hoc (Bonferroni)
H2a: Increased time spent on social media will lead to the user’s decreased overall
emotional well-being
Time
Time
Mean Difference
Std. Error
Sig.
0-10 min.
11-30 min.
.139
.070
.142
30+ min.
.333*
.096
.002
11-30 min.
0-10 min.
-.139
.070
.142
30+ min.
.195
.097
.135
30+ min.
0-10 min.
.333*
.096
.002
11-30 min.
-.195
.097
.135
N = 627; *p < .05
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 64
Table 12
Emotional well-being mediates the relationship between time/frequency and relationship quality
H3a (Time = X, Relationship Quality = Y, Emotional well-being = M)
1. X predicts M path A
a. F(1,625) = 11.8039, p = .0006
b. b = -.1601, t(625) = -3.4357, p = .0006
2. X and M together predicting Y
a. M predicts y path B
i. F(2,624) = 36.7820, p < .000
ii. b = 2.703, t(624) = 8.3454, p < .000
b. X no longer predicting Y path C
i. b = -.0264, t(624) = -.8192, p > .05
H3b (Frequency = X, Relationship Quality = Y, Emotional well-being = M)
1. X predicts M path A
a. F(1,625) = .6696, p = .4135
b. b = -.0402, t(625) = -.8183, p = .4135
2. X and M together predicting Y
a. M predicts y path B
i. F(2,624) = 37.1677, p < .000
ii. b = 2.733, t(624) = 8.5239, p < .000
b. X no longer predicting Y path C
i. b = -.0097, t(624) = -.2953, p > .05
N = 627
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 65
Table 13
RQ1: Does your use of social media influence your relationships? If so, how?
Category
# of responses
Negative results
Distracts from offline activities and/or people
80
Edgy/Irritated/Impatient/Jealous/Judging/Ignored/Want to escape
76
Less time spent together offline (quality time diminished)
52
Comparing relationships
42
Distrust/Avoid others
23
Relationship is not good enough/I want something better
22
I feel more isolated
21
Offline/Online relationships become superficial
20
Emotionally detached
15
Over-analyze/Misinterpret online activity
11
Unrealistic expectations (skewed perception of reality)
10
Ended relationship
8
Total (negative)
380
Positive results
Keep in touch
153
Strengthens relationship
62
Humor/Sharing
57
Offline communication increases based on topics viewed online
23
Happier in my relationships with less time on social media
20
Form new relationships
16
Total (positive)
331
Neutral results
Does not influence my relationships
218
Total (cumulative)
929
N = 643
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 66
Table 14
RQ2: Does your use of social media influence your emotions? If so, how?
Category
# of responses
Negative results
Frustrated/Annoyed/Irritated/Distracted/Stressed
153
Depressed/Sad
115
Social Comparison (negative)
107
Life dissatisfaction
84
Anger
57
Wasted Time (procrastination/lazy)
57
Envy/Jealousy
43
Apathetic/Empty/Numb
39
FOMO
39
Distorted perception of reality (others perfect lives)
38
Lonely
34
Anxiety
24
Increased isolation (less sociable offline)
18
Magnifies current emotions
18
Wanderlust
13
Total (negative)
841
Positive results
Happy/Positive
152
Humor
43
Happier with less time on Social Media
31
Support/Empathy
23
Mudita
23
Motivating/Inspiring
19
Validated/Accomplished
14
Calming/Relaxing/Relief
13
Fun
8
Total (positive)
326
Neutral results
Does not influence my emotions
127
Total (cumulative)
1,294
N = 668
SOCIAL MEDIA USE 67
Table 15
RQ3: Why do you use Social Media?
Category
# of responses
Connect with friends and family
461
News
188
Fun/Entertainment
176
Learn something new
91
Boredom
77
Kill/Waste time
59
Create/Share content
55
Work/Business
48
Escape
43
Participate in local events
25
Total
1,223
N = 680